The Role of Poverty Reduction in Baby’s First Years

What the science says about the connection between early childhood poverty and brain development in childhood

13 min

pexels-kha-ruxury-913179

A new study indicates that poverty reduction boosts a baby’s brain. The intervention, called Baby’s First Years, found that following a year of cash support, the brains of the 435 one-year-olds from families with lower incomes followed in the study registered higher cognitive skills.  

To learn more, we spoke about the findings with two authors of the paper, published in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences: Kimberly Noble, MD, PhD, Professor of Neuroscience and Education and Director, Developmental Psychology program, Teachers College at Columbia University and Lisa A. Gennetian, Ph.D., Pritzker Professor of Early Learning Policy Studies, Professor of Public Policy, Sanford School of Public Policy, Duke University. 

Tell me about reason for launching the Baby’s First Years study.  Kim: Scientists have known for many years that young children living in financially disadvantaged families and financially oppressed communities tend to be at risk for a host of outcomes, including lower school achievement, lower likelihood of employment, reduced earnings, and poorer health. Neuroscience research tells us that early childhood is a time when children’s brains are particularly sensitive to experience, and indeed, early childhood poverty has been associated with differences in children’s brain development.  

Neuroscience research tells us that early childhood is a time when children’s brains are particularly sensitive to experience, and indeed, early childhood poverty has been associated with differences in children’s brain development.

Kimberly Noble, MD, PhD, Professor of Neuroscience and Education and Director, Developmental Psychology program, Teachers College at Columbia University

However, whether living in poverty causes these outcomes, or is simply correlated with these outcomes, remains unclear. From a scientific perspective, the best way to answer that question is through a randomized controlled trial. That is the basis of the Baby's First Years (BFY) randomized control trial, in which we recruited 1,000 mothers living with low-income shortly after the birth of their child, and invited them to be randomly assigned to different levels of monthly cash support – either $333/month or $20/month. In this way, we can estimate the impact of our poverty reduction intervention on early childhood development, during a time when the developing brain is highly sensitive to differences in experience. 

Lisa:  I’ll also add that another companion and important impetus for this study is to fill a gap in the vast social science evidence base that to date has documented the unfavorable impacts of poverty on children’s development but not been able to speak to impacts over the course of children’s earliest years of development in part because of the challenges with measuring well-being during those early years.  This is where measures of brain activity offer a unique contribution.   

pexels-pixabay-161593

What has the study told us so far about how a poverty reduction intervention can impact infant brain activity?   Kim: First, allow me to provide some background on how we measure brain activity, and why we care about it. We can measure infant brain activity using a technology known as electroencephalography, or EEG. EEG lets us measure how quickly the brain is sending electrical signals between brain cells. All humans have some slow-paced brain activity and some fast-paced brain activity.  

In our study, we wanted to measure the speed of these brain activity signals in early childhood for three reasons: First, we know as children get older, they tend to have more fast-paced brain activity. Second, past research suggests that, on average, children with more fast-paced brain activity early in childhood tend to be more likely to score higher on tests of cognitive skills and other skills that are important for school. And third, past research has suggested that children growing up in poverty or facing other forms of early adversity often have less fast-paced brain activity early in childhood. 

Because of this, we reasoned that the monthly cash support delivered through our poverty reduction intervention would potentially mitigate that pattern. Specifically, we hypothesized that the infants of the mothers in the “high-cash gift group”—those receiving $333/month—would show more fast-paced brain activity and less slow-paced brain activity than the infants of the mothers in the “low-cash gift group,” or those receiving $20/month. The paper recently published in PNAS reports that, indeed, the infants of the mothers in the high-cash gift group are showing more fast-paced brain activity than the infants of the mothers in the low-cash gift group, particularly in key brain regions that support the development of thinking and learning.  

Because of the pandemic, we obtained brain activity data from only about half of the sample that we originally anticipated. That meant that, while all the findings on fast-paced brain activity were in the direction we anticipated, some, but not all of those findings, were statistically significant. Putting together all of the evidence from the various analyses we conducted, we concluded that the weight of the evidence supported the conclusion that monthly unconditional cash support given to families early in childhood can change children’s brain activity, in a pattern associated with the development of subsequent thinking and learning. 

the weight of the evidence supported the conclusion that monthly unconditional cash support given to families early in childhood can change children’s brain activity, in a pattern associated with the development of subsequent thinking and learning.

Kimberly Noble, MD, PhD, Professor of Neuroscience and Education and Director, Developmental Psychology program, Teachers College at Columbia University

What implications could the study have for the field?  Lisa: This research is designed to fill a fundamental gap in social science and neuroscience research. On the one hand, decades of social science research point to the negative effects of poverty on children’s development, and its corollary, the benefits of social policies that offer economic support to eligible families. This existing evidence can speak especially well to older children, starting around the time of preschool, and in some cases, to outcomes measured at birth, but less well to an increasingly recognized important period of early development, from birth to age three. As mentioned earlier, this is in part because of how hard it is to get objective measures of children’s development during those early years.  

This research is designed to fill a fundamental gap in social science and neuroscience research.

Lisa A. Gennetian, Ph.D., Pritzker Professor of Early Learning Policy Studies, Professor of Public Policy, Sanford School of Public Policy, Duke University

At the same time, the last decade or so, the neuroscience literature has been generating a body of work showing correlations between children’s early environments, including socio-economic status of households, and brain activity, in early childhood. This research however cannot untangle cause from correlation, leaving open the question of how much poverty reduction, or income per se, impacts children’s brain functioning. 

This study emerged to answer this gap in the literature, for example how do we go about trying to induce a change in family poverty in a defensible way that can answer the causal question?  

Can you talk a little about how findings could practically serve parents and caregivers on the ground level?  Lisa: At the time of designing and launching this study (in 2018-19) the U.S. did not have safety net or related income-support policies designed to increase family net income without conditions (like conditioned on being employed or having earnings) or not targeted in some way (including food or housing assistance). There are many reasons for this but one of those is related to efficiency (so, if reducing hunger is the goal then providing food stamps is the more efficient solution); yet another is trusting families residing in poverty to spend money in ways that is best for their family and their children.  

This study turns this around and is designed from the perspective of honoring parent agency and enabling families who live in poverty to spend money in ways that can be most supportive without a dictate or requirement or limitation or judgment. This lens, we hope, is an empowering perspective for parents and caregivers. 

Are there any potential policy implications based on the study’s findings?  Lisa: The U.S. stands apart from its peer nations in not having a fully refundable child benefit.  We hope that the evidence from this study will inform considerations of such a child benefit.

U.S. stands apart from its peer nations in not having a fully refundable child benefit.  We hope that the evidence from this study will inform considerations of such a child benefit.

Lisa A. Gennetian, Ph.D., Pritzker Professor of Early Learning Policy Studies, Professor of Public Policy, Sanford School of Public Policy, Duke University

Income or cash support alone is not enough to address child poverty in the U.S. nor the only ingredient important to support children’s development; one important feature of BFY is the efforts we put into reducing the likelihood that families become ineligible for other benefits and supports that are means-tested; in fact, one hypothesis we’ve considered is how stable monthly income might give families more room (for example attention) to engage with community and other supports and benefits.   

The BFY cash gift has many features that mimic the recently expanded child tax credit. There are also important differences. One key difference is that the expanded child tax credit is for every child in the household under the age 17; the BFY cash gift is one monthly amount irrespective of the number of children. 

Finally, one way that this study’s cash is designed that is implicit but incredibly unique is its certainty and predictability; and second, its guarantee irrespective of changing circumstances, so it is a promise of a monthly cash gift for at least 4 years with no questions asked and no strings attached.   

What findings will come next and when those will be released?  Lisa: We’ve collected a lot of information about family life and mother’s well-being, including key information on family expenditures on items like childcare and early enriching items like books and toys; as well as mother’s time spent with the child, breastfeeding, and time in employment. The analyses are in progress with many papers in the pipeline.   

Kim: A lot of time is being devoted to preparing for the upcoming capstone wave of data collection. We are planning for a resumption of in-person data collection, beginning around the children's fourth birthdays. We are going to be inviting the families into our university research settings to once again be able to measure brain activity—hopefully with a much larger sample—and also, most importantly, we will be engaging in direct assessments of children’s cognitive and behavioral development. It will be an opportunity to measure the impact of regular, monthly unconditional cash support for the first four years of life on direct measures of child development. 

 

Bezos Family Foundation is proud to support an extension of the Baby’s First Years study, which is the first randomized controlled trial of poverty reduction in early childhood in the U.S. Findings from the study will inform state and national policymaking across a range of safety net and income support programs for generations.